Skip to main content

Jamais Vu: Scientists Probe the Uncanny Twin of Déjà Vu and What It Reveals About the Human Mind

4 min read
862 words
Share:

In a discovery that stretches the boundaries of memory research, scientists have recently shed light on “jamais vu”—the rare and unsettling feeling that something deeply familiar suddenly appears strange or unrecognizable. While most Thais are familiar with the spine-tingling phenomenon of déjà vu, the eerie sense that one has lived through a moment before, its lesser-known counterpart, jamais vu, proves to be even more uncanny, and researchers say it holds important clues to the workings of the human brain (ScienceAlert).

The recent study, which garnered an Ig Nobel Prize for literature, demonstrates that jamais vu can be intentionally induced in the laboratory simply by asking participants to repeat a word, such as “door” or “the,” over and over again. After about a minute of repetition, nearly 70% of subjects reported a sense of unfamiliarity so profound that they voluntarily stopped, citing feelings like “it doesn’t seem right, almost looks like it’s not really a word but someone’s tricked me into thinking it is.” Such findings underscore jamais vu’s potential as a window into our cognitive reality-checking systems—mechanisms that help ensure we aren’t lost in meaningless routine.

For Thai readers, this research draws attention to aspects of cognition that are not only universal but resonate with uniquely local experiences. Whether it’s a student practicing the same kata in Thai classical dance, an office worker entering the same BTS station daily, or a Buddhist practitioner reciting familiar mantras, the repetitive nature of some tasks might occasionally give way to moments of jamais vu—a temporary but jarring sense that the habitual has become alien.

Researchers behind the study asked 94 undergraduates to write common words repeatedly, noting that three quarters of participants eventually stopped due to the uncanny sensation—the hallmark of jamais vu. The effect typically set in after about 33 repetitions, with more familiar words often inducing the response faster. In a second experiment employing only the word “the,” more than half of participants again reported jamais vu, but this time after around 27 repetitions. These simple setups mirror day-to-day experiences where monotony can provoke a dissociative sense of unreality.

Experts explain that this phenomenon is not new—its echo can be traced back over a century. In 1907, early psychologists described how words stared at for several minutes lost their meaning and fragmented perceptually. The latest research, however, highlights “satiation”—the overloading of mental representations, causing them to lose meaning and coherence—as the underlying mechanism. This process, scientists argue, is a crucial safeguard: when perception becomes too automatic, jamais vu acts as an adaptive reality check. It’s the mind’s way of snapping us out of autopilot and regaining control over attention.

From a Thai cultural perspective, the phenomenon might provide fresh insight into common discomforts during prolonged meditation or repetitive prayer. Similarly, students studying for exams may recognize the sensation: when a word written over and over—such as “appetite” (ความอยากอาหาร)—suddenly appears unfamiliar or misspelled, even though the spelling is correct. Thai musicians practicing familiar passages may momentarily lose their place, experiencing music as alien instead of rote. Such episodes are more than eccentric curiosities; they’re vital reminders of how flexible and self-monitoring our cognitive systems are.

Supporting this interpretation, research into related conditions such as obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) finds that individuals compulsively checking locks or gas handles may enter a jamais vu-like state where reality “slips,” making it harder to be sure whether the task has been performed. This runs the risk of trapping them in an anxious cycle of constant re-checking. The cognitive neuroscience behind jamais vu not only spotlights ordinary experiences but may also illuminate paths to treatment for those whose reality-checking systems have gone awry (The Conversation).

Recent findings also relate to the “verbal transformation effect,” where repeating a word, such as “tress,” ends with listeners perceiving entirely new words—“dress,” “stress,” or “florist.” This cognitive flexibility, while producing bizarre sensations in the short term, supports our mind’s adaptability and creativity over the long term.

For a country with rich traditions of rote learning, chanting, and rehearsal—whether in monastic, musical, or martial settings—understanding jamais vu may highlight the value, as well as the psychological risk, of repetition. Awareness of this phenomenon can be used to refine educational and mindfulness practices in Thai classrooms and temples, encouraging periodic breaks, variation, and reflection to avoid cognitive overload.

As for future research, neuroscientists emphasize that jamais vu remains woefully understudied compared to its more famous sibling, déjà vu. Larger studies are needed to tease out individual, cultural, and linguistic differences in susceptibility, with the hope that understanding “reality-check failures” can be channeled into better mental health interventions.

For Thai students, teachers, and workers, a simple practice can help harness the positive side of jamais vu: when the familiar turns strange through repetition, see it as your mind’s invitation to pause, breathe, and reconnect purposefully with the task at hand. Embracing variety in routine, and occasionally stepping back, can protect against mental fatigue and cultivate a fresher, more grounded perspective.

For more information and further reading:

Related Articles

5 min read

Decoding Déjà Vu: Understanding the Brain’s Fleeting Sense of Familiarity

news psychology

A recent article from Psychology Today has reignited scientific curiosity about déjà vu, that uncanny moment when a new experience feels eerily familiar—even when logic insists it shouldn’t be. This fleeting sense, known in French as “already seen,” has long puzzled neuroscientists and psychologists. Now, emerging research offers a richer understanding of déjà vu’s origins and its surprising role in human cognition, raising questions for Thai readers about how their own brains handle memory and experience PsychologyToday.com.

#neuroscience #memory #psychology +7 more
5 min read

New Research Reveals Four Key Psychological Markers Linked to Ideological Extremism

news psychology

A new wave of scientific research is shedding light on the psychological and neurobiological roots of ideological extremism, revealing that certain mental traits may make people more susceptible to radical beliefs. According to recent findings discussed in Big Think, renowned researcher Dr. Leor Zmigrod, author of the book “The Ideological Brain,” has identified four principal psychological markers that correlate closely with extremist ideologies: cognitive rigidity, emotional volatility, differences in the amygdala, and structural traits within the prefrontal cortex.

#ideologicalextremism #psychology #Thailand +7 more
5 min read

Misokinesia: The Little-Known Condition Disrupting Lives of One Third of People

news psychology

A recent wave of scientific research has brought attention to a previously little-known condition called misokinesia—an aversion to the sight of others’ repetitive movements—which is now estimated to affect nearly one in three people worldwide. Emerging evidence indicates that this visual sensitivity is as pervasive as it is misunderstood, profoundly influencing social interactions and mental well-being, including within Thai society.

Misokinesia, a term translating literally as “hatred of movement,” refers to the strong negative emotional responses—ranging from mild annoyance to intense psychological distress—that some individuals experience when witnessing small, repetitive movements performed by others. Examples include finger tapping, leg bouncing, pen clicking, fidgeting with objects, chewing motions, or nervous tics (Futura Sciences). While the concept may be unfamiliar to the general public, new research led by psychologists at the University of British Columbia (UBC) and published in Nature/Scientific Reports in 2021 has shown it is far from rare.

#misokinesia #mentalhealth #neuroscience +7 more

Medical Disclaimer: This article is for informational purposes only and should not be considered medical advice. Always consult with qualified healthcare professionals before making decisions about your health.