Skip to main content

Neuroscientist Argues 16-Year-Olds Have the Cognitive Skills to Vote: Should Thailand Follow Suit?

5 min read
1,187 words
Share:

A leading neuroscientist is reigniting debate over the voting age by arguing that 16-year-olds possess the cognitive skills needed to cast ballots responsibly, citing both neurological development and international examples as evidence. The discussion, sparked by a recent article in The Times, is gaining traction in countries considering electoral reform—including those, like Thailand, where youth political engagement has surged in recent years.

At a time when several democracies are weighing whether to lower their voting ages, neuroscientific research offers powerful new insights. According to the neuroscientist interviewed by The Times, teenagers aged 16 can reason, reflect, and make considered decisions—skills grounded in “cold cognition,” or logical thinking in calm settings. These cognitive abilities, the expert explains, develop enough by mid-adolescence to underpin mature choices at the ballot box.

The article clarifies that adolescents are indeed more susceptible to risky behavior in the impulsive, emotionally charged landscapes described as “hot cognition.” Yet, voting is typically a private, deliberate act, insulated from immediate peer or emotional pressures. Therefore, while teenagers may take more risks among friends, the voting process is not comparable to decision-making in social or high-adrenaline contexts.

Despite surging interest in lowering voting ages, international practice has been cautious. Some countries—namely Austria—have already extended voting rights to 16- and 17-year-olds. Notably, turnout rates for first-time voters in these age groups often exceed those among 18- to 20-year-olds, according to electoral studies drawing on data from Austria and the 2014 Scottish independence referendum. Results suggest that when young people are introduced to voting earlier, they are more likely to develop habits of lifelong democratic participation (Oxford Academic; The Conversation). The trend is supported by further evidence indicating that those who begin voting at 16 are more likely to continue participating in elections throughout their twenties compared with those who start at 18.

The neuroscientist, however, cautions against viewing brain development as a fixed milestone: “Brain development does not stop at 16—or even at 18 or 25. There is no precise moment when the brain becomes ‘adult’.” Rather, cognitive development is gradual, and individual variation means some 15-year-olds may show greater maturity than some 18-year-olds. Still, legal and social settings routinely require broad age thresholds, and neuroscience can inform, but not definitively select, these boundaries.

Opponents of lowering the voting age often cite restrictions on activities such as purchasing alcohol or driving, but the neuroscientist rejects these comparisons as misleading. Each legal age, they argue, is set for different reasons—often linked to health or safety risks, not responsible decision-making. Crucially, in the United Kingdom, 16-year-olds can already make significant societal contributions: they may join the military, pay taxes, and face criminal responsibility from age 10 (12 in Scotland). If these responsibilities are entrusted to teenagers, why not voting?

The conversation is especially relevant in a Thai context, as the country is witnessing historic levels of youth civic engagement. Mass student-led protests demanding political reform between 2020–2022 signaled a generational shift (Bangkok Post), with teenagers, including many under 18, taking center stage in public discourse. Thailand’s Constitution currently grants suffrage at age 18, but some academics and policymakers are beginning to consider international debates and local youth activism as reasons to revisit this limit.

A senior psychologist at a leading Thai public university, who specializes in adolescent cognition, points out that “the core neurological functions for assessing consequences and complex problem-solving are well in place by age 16 for most youth.” This is echoed by a policy researcher at a Bangkok think tank, who notes that “civic knowledge and motivation among Thai teens now arguably matches, or exceeds, that of many adults.” The nation’s last general election saw a spike in youth turnout and subsequent engagement across digital platforms (The Nation Thailand).

Thai cultural attitudes toward age and maturity, however, remain deeply anchored to notions of hierarchy and “phu yai–phu noi” (literally, elder–younger) roles in society. Traditionalists often argue that political voice should be reserved for those considered fully mature. A prominent educator and cultural commentator recently stated in an interview that “granting voting rights to 16-year-olds could disrupt Thailand’s delicate social balance and erode respect for senior wisdom,” echoing concerns among older generations.

Still, international evidence could challenge these assumptions. The Austrian case provides a compelling example: not only did younger voters prove responsible, but their early participation was associated with higher motivation and deeper civic identity. Both psychology and political science literature support the view that adolescence is a crucial time to foster democratic habits (British Journal of Political Science). When the Thai government allows 16-year-olds to make consequential decisions—be it working, paying taxes, or being subject to criminal law—it may logically follow to also extend the vote.

Should Thailand lower its voting age, potential impacts could include a revamping of civic education in secondary schools, enhanced focus on social and political content in the curriculum, and the design of targeted voter outreach for youth. Educational reformers have called for strengthening critical thinking and participatory skills from lower secondary levels, arguing that this would complement any future expansion of voting rights.

Opponents warn against rushing into reform. A senior official within the Election Commission of Thailand, when questioned on the possibility, stated: “Any policy change must be matched by robust civic education and safeguards against manipulation or disinformation, which can disproportionately affect younger voters.” Recent concerns around social media misinformation and the vulnerability of all age groups—especially youth—to digital campaigns support such caution (Reuters).

Longstanding Thai cultural values—such as the importance of respect for elders and family consultation—may also shape how lower voting ages are accepted socially. Historically, age-based hierarchies have influenced not only household decision-making but also broader institutions, from school classrooms to parliament chambers. As Thailand negotiates the interplay between tradition and democratic pluralism, this global debate on teenage suffrage cuts to the heart of the kingdom’s social contract.

Looking ahead, the neuroscience-based perspective is likely to fuel further research and discussion. If Thailand were to move toward considering 16-year-olds as potential voters, policymakers, educators, and community leaders would need to weigh neural development, civic readiness, social context, and the evolving role of youth in national life. As the neuroscientist concludes, “We need a fair and inclusive system that respects competence and nurtures young people’s engagement with democracy”—a message resonating far beyond the UK.

For Thai readers, this debate offers timely questions to ponder. Should young people who can already shape society through taxes, military service, and work also help shape the kingdom’s future at the ballot box? What steps can schools, families, and communities take to prepare youth for informed, reflective decision-making? As Thai society evolves, ensuring that democratic systems are both inclusive and robust—grounded in scientific understanding and cultural wisdom—will be key.

For concerned parents, educators, and policymakers, the actionable first step is to promote youth civic education, create spaces for respectful political dialogue, and actively monitor international research and policy outcomes. Supporting young Thais to develop critical reasoning and civic confidence may be the nation’s best investment, regardless of how the voting age debate unfolds.

Sources: The Times, Oxford Academic, The Conversation, Bangkok Post, The Nation Thailand, British Journal of Political Science, Reuters

Related Articles

4 min read

Reading or Listening: Neuroscience Reveals Key Differences in How We Learn

news neuroscience

A groundbreaking review in neuroscience is challenging commonly held beliefs about how reading and listening shape what we learn, revealing that each medium activates the brain in unique ways and is not simply interchangeable. While today’s digital landscape overflows with audiobooks, podcasts, and voice-driven AI, new research highlights that reading in print or on screens still plays an irreplaceable role in effective learning—especially when grappling with complex material (The Conversation, 2025).

#reading #audiobooks #neuroscience +5 more
5 min read

Brainwaves in Harmony: New Study Reveals Why Optimists “Think Alike”

news neuroscience

A groundbreaking study by a Japanese research team has revealed that optimists’ brains display strikingly similar patterns of neural activity when imagining the future, while pessimists’ neural signatures are much more unique and individualistic. Published in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), the findings illuminate how our outlook on life is physically manifested in the mind—and may help explain why optimists excel at building social relationships (medicalxpress.com).

#optimism #neuroscience #mentalhealth +7 more
4 min read

Advanced Microscopy Sheds New Light on Dopamine’s Surgical Precision in the Brain

news neuroscience

A groundbreaking new study is challenging the conventional wisdom about dopamine, a crucial brain chemical long believed to broadcast broad, sweeping signals throughout the brain. Using advanced microscopy techniques, researchers have revealed that dopamine may actually operate with remarkable surgical precision, finely targeting specific brain cells rather than acting as an indiscriminate messenger. This discovery offers significant new insights into how the brain controls movement, motivation, and learning—areas that are especially relevant to Thai readers interested in neurological health and the future of treatments for conditions such as Parkinson’s disease, addiction, and depression.

#neuroscience #dopamine #Thailand +6 more

Medical Disclaimer: This article is for informational purposes only and should not be considered medical advice. Always consult with qualified healthcare professionals before making decisions about your health.